Climate Change and the Amazon Drought |
The big problem with most climate change speeches
is that the speakers tell the audience that there is a lot of evidence about
climate change, but they don’t explain what the evidence is. I
showed in my previous post that you can’t persuade climate change skeptics by
insulting them and calling them names. You also can’t persuade climate change skeptics
just by promising that the scientists agree. That asks them to change their minds on
faith, and skeptics are not going to change their minds on faith. They need
evidence. That doesn’t mean that evidence will always persuade them; it won’t.
But evidence is all we have.
Persuasion
Theory
Let’s go over a few basic points of persuasion
theory. A well-established persuasion theory is the Elaboration Likelihood Model, which I have written about
a few times. This theory, developed by psychologists Richard E. Petty and John
Cacioppo, says that there are two paths by which listeners process persuasive
messages. People follow the central path
when they think about a message carefully. This occurs when people are
motivated to analyze a persuasive message and feel that they have the skill and
knowledge to do so. The peripheral path
does not involve careful thinking. Instead, the listener relies on peripheral cues, such as whether the listener likes the speaker, finds the
speaker attractive, or already has a positive attitude toward the message. The
number (as opposed to the quality) of messages gives listeners a peripheral
cue.
When people change their attitudes over the
central route, the attitude change is likely to be long-lasting and to
influence their behavior. For example, it might influence their long-term
voting. But attitude change that occurs over the peripheral route is likely to
be temporary and easily changed. Furthermore, attitude change over the
peripheral route is unlikely to change people’s long-term behavior. Much
advertising, including most political advertising, aims at the peripheral
route. That, for example, is why many political messages are ridiculously
short and convey
little actual information. Voters who process information on a given topic
by the central route will ignore such short, simple messages.
Speakers cannot control whether the audience will
process a message by the central or peripheral route. They can, however, offer
the audience opportunities to process information. When a speaker says, “scientists
agree about climate change,” the speaker is appealing purely to the peripheral
route. That almost
all scientists do, in fact, agree about climate change is a peripheral cue. Climate change skeptics,
however, are exposed to many other peripheral cues. For example, they
hear Fox
News hosts and talk
radio personalities call climate change a hoax. When speakers give the
audience too little information, they offer the audience little chance to
process a message by the central route. Of course, a highly motivated audience member can find the research on Google Scholar. But how many people will do so? Climate change speakers make a mistake when
they do not invite central route processing. Here are the reasons why and what to do about them:
First, the evidence for climate change is complex.
A superficial speech cannot address complexities.
Second, audience members might not feel that they have the knowledge or ability
to analyze the scientific information for themselves. People use the central
route only when they feel motivated and
capable. That is why it becomes a speaker’s job to present information in a way
that is accessible and persuasive. That requires a speaker to do research (and
I’ve often commented that speakers
need research) and to present that research to the audience in a way that
is clear and well-organized. That requires a speaker to make a lot of effort,
but the effort might pay off in a higher likelihood that the audience will
choose to process the information critically.
Third, the speaker needs to give the audience a
reason to think carefully about climate change. The central route requires effort! That is where Greta Thunberg has been
doing a great job. She is enthusiastic and knows how to get the audience’s
attention. But what she has not done, however, is to help people understand the
information. That’s probably too much to expect from any 16-year-old speaker,
no matter how smart she is, so it’s time for other speakers to pick up the load
and take the next step.
Fourth, it is necessary to anticipate the most
common objections that climate change skeptics present. Skeptics will say that
scientists once complained about global cooling, which is partially true. They
will point out that there are natural causes of climate change. This is also
true, although a bit beside the point. Some people will flat-out deny that the
climate is changing at all, since day-to day weather changes much more than the
two degrees of warming that we’ve seen so far from climate change. Speakers should not
expect conservative objectors to give them a reasonable chance to engage in
back-and-forth dialogue. That experience, which high school and college debate
teams take for granted, rarely occurs in the real world. Instead, speakers need
to anticipate and preempt the major objections, and to refute those objections
with care. Speakers should assume that they only get one shot to make their case. (That doesn't mean that one speech is enough, but that's a topic for another day.) Again, asking the audience to take something on faith invites them
to process the message by the peripheral route.
All effective persuasion comes down to giving people information. The better job
a speaker does of presenting information, the more likely it is that audiences
will respond. We ask a lot of people when we expect them to cut back how much they use the fossil-fuel
energy sources that have made the industrial nations so prosperous. It is wrong
to think that changing people’s minds will ever be an easy job. Any time we think about persuasion, we
need to think about the audience.
So, that’s what persuasion research tells us. In
my next post, I’ll show how these persuasion principles might work in a
practical way.
P.S. Many good sources explain the Elaboration Likelihood Model. Here’s a simple, accurate explanation
by an Oregon State University professor. Any current persuasion
textbook will also discuss this model. Ambitious (and wealthy) readers can
buy an e-book version of Petty and Cacioppo’s book here.
Image from NASA.
No comments:
Post a Comment